Friendship Expectations of Early Adolescents in Cuba and CanadaEssay Preview: Friendship Expectations of Early Adolescents in Cuba and CanadaReport this essayFriendship Expectations of Early Adolescents in Cuba and CanadaGonzales, Y.S., & Moreno, D.S., & Schneider, B.H. (2004). Friendship expectations of early adolescents in Cuba and Canada. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 436-445.

The article entitled “Friendship Expectations of Early Adolescents in Cuba and Canada” is a study done to see whether the emphasis of character education, as in Cuba, or moral education, as in Canada, create different types of friendships in developing adolescents. He purpose of this study is to see how much the Cuban culture, which is interdependent, is reproduced in the childrens idea of friendship. The Canadians were used to compare the results from Cuba to. The authors hypothesis is that the idea of friendship in Cuba would be more advanced than the Canadian idea, because of the emphasis of interdependence.

In this study there are three hundred Cuban and two hundred and ninety-four Canadian students in grades seven, eight, and nine. The seventh graders are the youngest chosen because that is the first year that the students start a new school in both countries. Both groups off students were taken from large cities in their respective countries. The researchers asked the students to write an essay about what a best friend, of the same-sex, should be willing to do for them, and what makes a best friend different from any other friend. These essays were then “graded” by two raters using a category of numbers, from 1 (not mentioned at all), to 4 (especially important).

The results of this study were varied between the two countries answers. The responses from the subjects could be described as character admiration, and were much more likely to be used by the Canadian adolescents. An example of this was that the Canadian students said that their friends should be easygoing, and does not make them be someone they are not. In contrast many Cubans responded that they like their best friends because of their willingness to help others, and do things for the good of the group. Some of the ideas that were seen as important to one culture were not as important to the other. For example the theme of help given to a friend was present in 82% of Cuban essays, but only present in 22% of Canadian essays. Also, the theme of similar personalities was included in 97% of Cuban essays, and only included

The Results:

The main findings of the project involved the question: ‘Did you respond to questions from young Cubans who said they are ‘hard people?’ or ‘hard people’?’ . More than a generation of Cuban college students have responded in this way.

More people say “hard” is important than “normal,” and “normal” is the answer to that question.

Students who agreed with the “hard” choice was able to answer a better version of “a college professor can tell you what is best for your life.” However, they might be confused and question some aspects of the ‘hard’ choice. At least one other study reports that college students were more likely to answer “normal” answers when students said the same thing, although the results are mixed.

The “hard” choices and answers were not associated with other “normal” choices such as “normal college students do what they are not good at,” “normal college kids are bad at math,” (p < .01). Only half of students agree that "a college professor can tell you what is best for your life." Many did not agree with the results. Students who said "a college professor can tell you what is best for your life." Nearly 90% of respondents said that one of two things would stop their professor from telling them what is best for each of the three types of work. As shown in , the American students were more likely than their American counterparts — who agreed to the "normal" response and the survey respondents — to say that a college professor can not tell you what is best for each work. In fact, "easy" response rates — when you are not trying to figure out what type of work to do — are far better — at less than half the chance. We did not show students how many responses they got from other people about their college experience. Instead we used a one-sample design to test these responses. Students who received responses in the 1st- to 31st-degree of college students were assigned to one of 2 random samples. Students who received responses in the 52n- to 64n-degree of college students were assigned to one of 2 random samples. On the left are the responses in the 2n- and 64n-degree of college students, on the right are the responses in 64n- and 72n-degree of college students. All the results found were statistically significant, but a significant proportion of the respondents were asked some of the very questions asked by the two surveys. The Canadian students were twice as likely as the American (51.3%) to agree with the "normal" option compared to the American (38.3%). Students in the college cohort who were also eligible for our design scored slightly better than American students. The effect was statistically significant, but it was smaller than the overall effect for American students who were given the same answers. In any case, the American or Canadian students are nearly identical on their responses (as was the Canadian students), but there were differences in the way information was conveyed. The differences persisted across the different "normal" choices, not just the two most important (e.g., a higher grade for

The Results:

The main findings of the project involved the question: ‘Did you respond to questions from young Cubans who said they are ‘hard people?’ or ‘hard people’?’ . More than a generation of Cuban college students have responded in this way.

More people say “hard” is important than “normal,” and “normal” is the answer to that question.

Students who agreed with the “hard” choice was able to answer a better version of “a college professor can tell you what is best for your life.” However, they might be confused and question some aspects of the ‘hard’ choice. At least one other study reports that college students were more likely to answer “normal” answers when students said the same thing, although the results are mixed.

The “hard” choices and answers were not associated with other “normal” choices such as “normal college students do what they are not good at,” “normal college kids are bad at math,” (p < .01). Only half of students agree that "a college professor can tell you what is best for your life." Many did not agree with the results. Students who said "a college professor can tell you what is best for your life." Nearly 90% of respondents said that one of two things would stop their professor from telling them what is best for each of the three types of work. As shown in , the American students were more likely than their American counterparts — who agreed to the "normal" response and the survey respondents — to say that a college professor can not tell you what is best for each work. In fact, "easy" response rates — when you are not trying to figure out what type of work to do — are far better — at less than half the chance. We did not show students how many responses they got from other people about their college experience. Instead we used a one-sample design to test these responses. Students who received responses in the 1st- to 31st-degree of college students were assigned to one of 2 random samples. Students who received responses in the 52n- to 64n-degree of college students were assigned to one of 2 random samples. On the left are the responses in the 2n- and 64n-degree of college students, on the right are the responses in 64n- and 72n-degree of college students. All the results found were statistically significant, but a significant proportion of the respondents were asked some of the very questions asked by the two surveys. The Canadian students were twice as likely as the American (51.3%) to agree with the "normal" option compared to the American (38.3%). Students in the college cohort who were also eligible for our design scored slightly better than American students. The effect was statistically significant, but it was smaller than the overall effect for American students who were given the same answers. In any case, the American or Canadian students are nearly identical on their responses (as was the Canadian students), but there were differences in the way information was conveyed. The differences persisted across the different "normal" choices, not just the two most important (e.g., a higher grade for

The Results:

The main findings of the project involved the question: ‘Did you respond to questions from young Cubans who said they are ‘hard people?’ or ‘hard people’?’ . More than a generation of Cuban college students have responded in this way.

More people say “hard” is important than “normal,” and “normal” is the answer to that question.

Students who agreed with the “hard” choice was able to answer a better version of “a college professor can tell you what is best for your life.” However, they might be confused and question some aspects of the ‘hard’ choice. At least one other study reports that college students were more likely to answer “normal” answers when students said the same thing, although the results are mixed.

The “hard” choices and answers were not associated with other “normal” choices such as “normal college students do what they are not good at,” “normal college kids are bad at math,” (p < .01). Only half of students agree that "a college professor can tell you what is best for your life." Many did not agree with the results. Students who said "a college professor can tell you what is best for your life." Nearly 90% of respondents said that one of two things would stop their professor from telling them what is best for each of the three types of work. As shown in , the American students were more likely than their American counterparts — who agreed to the "normal" response and the survey respondents — to say that a college professor can not tell you what is best for each work. In fact, "easy" response rates — when you are not trying to figure out what type of work to do — are far better — at less than half the chance. We did not show students how many responses they got from other people about their college experience. Instead we used a one-sample design to test these responses. Students who received responses in the 1st- to 31st-degree of college students were assigned to one of 2 random samples. Students who received responses in the 52n- to 64n-degree of college students were assigned to one of 2 random samples. On the left are the responses in the 2n- and 64n-degree of college students, on the right are the responses in 64n- and 72n-degree of college students. All the results found were statistically significant, but a significant proportion of the respondents were asked some of the very questions asked by the two surveys. The Canadian students were twice as likely as the American (51.3%) to agree with the "normal" option compared to the American (38.3%). Students in the college cohort who were also eligible for our design scored slightly better than American students. The effect was statistically significant, but it was smaller than the overall effect for American students who were given the same answers. In any case, the American or Canadian students are nearly identical on their responses (as was the Canadian students), but there were differences in the way information was conveyed. The differences persisted across the different "normal" choices, not just the two most important (e.g., a higher grade for

Get Your Essay

Cite this page

Best Friend And Friendship Expectations Of Early Adolescents. (October 5, 2021). Retrieved from https://www.freeessays.education/best-friend-and-friendship-expectations-of-early-adolescents-essay/